Autism and Epistemic Injustice
'In what ways are autistic people seen as ‘other’? Epistemic injustice, neuro norms and white supremacy; the role of autistic people in a pluralistic society.'
Sections:
1. Background to autism.
2. Autism and the neurodiversity movement.
3. ‘Othering’ and epistemic injustice
(include readings from course).
4. Autism in educational settings.
5. Pluralism and acceptance, the role of recognising and accepting autistic identity.
1. Background to autism.
The American Psychiatric Association definition is as follows:
‘Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex developmental condition involving persistent challenges with social communication, restricted interests, and repetitive behavior. While autism is considered a lifelong disorder, the degree of impairment in functioning because of these challenges varies between individuals with autism.’ (American Psychiatric Association, 2023)
It is important to note that the definition above originates from a medical model which characterises and diagnoses autism via behavioural traits that are seen as ‘other’ to societal norms. This frame of reference is key to understanding perceptions of autism within society as a whole. Autism research, as a body of work, has been led in the main by non-autistic researchers, furthermore many organisations who fundraise or offer supports for autism, have centred non-autistic voices in their policy making or publicity campaigns. This is an example of epistemic violence as defined by Dotson, 2011) who says that
'epistemic violence in testimony is a refusal, intentional or unintentional, of an audience to communicatively reciprocate a linguistic exchange owing to pernicious ignorance.’
This exclusion of autistic voices has been widely
criticised by the autistic and neurodivergent community, as has the ‘medical
model’ with many autistic people advocating for language that is less
medicalised and more accepting of difference. The language itself, which often
presents autistic traits as ‘deficits’ or ‘problems’ frequently creates stigma
by its very nature. This medical model
is referred to in Lindblom, (2017) where she writes that ‘this way
of thinking does not fit within Indigenous worldviews, in which all people are
valued for who they are.’ This connects
to the wider place of autistic people within a colonial model of thinking and
the value systems which create norms of behaviour and being.
Particular
characteristics of autistic people, such as body movements which self-regulate,
differences in eye contact and direct communications styles are seen as not
fitting into standard models of comportment.
Importantly, many traditional theories of autism are based on
behavioural observations in which the neurotypical researchers use observance
of phenomena as the entire means of ‘naming’ autism. This lack of examination of underlying dynamics
and forces inevitably produces an incomplete picture (Kourti, 2021).
Many
autistic people would argue that a number of the disabling features of autism
are due to lack of understanding and lack of adequate accommodations. This is the premise of the social model of
disability. This social model is very
pertinent to the autistic experience; however it may simplify a complex set of
interactions between our role as social beings, and the physical reality of bodily
or psychological impairments. Catala,
Faucher and Poirier, (2021) discuss this a little more and offer an
‘interactionist’ model as a way of addressing some of these difficulties.
‘The interactionist model of disability as disablement dovetails nicely with the model of agency …in which ability is construed as the result of a dynamic interaction between an agent’s skills and the features afforded by their environment’.
The term
neurodiversity was coined in 1998 by Judy Singer, who was an Australian
sociologist, and this term was quickly adopted by the autistic community. Per Autistic
UK, (2023),
‘Neurodiversity is the diversity of human brains and minds, the infinite variation in neurocognitive functioning within our species. The Neurodiversity Paradigm is an emergent paradigm in which neurodiversity is understood to be a form of human diversity that is subject to the same social dynamics as other forms of diversity (including dynamics of power and oppression).’
The
neurodiversity movement originated as a grassroots movement and has gained
momentum since its beginnings, with increased recognition of the voices of
autistic people and with the input of autistic led research and advocacy. Neurodiversity does not simply refer to
autistic people, but is a wide umbrella which encompasses conditions like ADHD,
dyspraxia, dyscalculia, amongst others. The movement is closely linked with the
disability movement and advocates for de-stigmatisation of autism (and other
brain differences), and for greater acceptance and societal supports. According to Tougaw, (2020),
‘Philosophically, the neurodiversity movement is based on what we might call cerebral pluralism—the idea that each brain is different and some more different than others.’
3. ‘Othering’
and epistemic injustice.
Autistic
people are ‘othered’ in multiple ways. As with other disabilities, autistic
people can be seen as different and less able to ‘perform’ in a society which
values economic productivity so highly. Media portrayals) often represent
autistic people as robotic, lacking empathy, unable to socialise, and largely
are represented as male. There are two common
tropes, the autistic non speaking child who presents as being severely disabled
and requiring a high level of care, or the quirky clever scientist type who
remains different, but nonetheless in a more acceptable form. This latter character is frequently manifested
in the form of ‘geek’ or autistic coded characters like Sheldon Cooper or
Sherlock Holmes. (Shepard, 2010) speaks of media representations and says that ‘…these personas are also so often masculine, middle/upper class and
white. These representations are problematic in the way that they uphold
traditional normativity in terms of gender, race and class, as well as reifying
stigma toward other points on the autistic spectrum.’
Within this normative sphere, ‘Aspie Supremacy’ is something which is
often critiqued within the autistic community.
The term derives from what was a category of Autism, Asperger’s
Syndrome, which was subsequently removed from the DSM (diagnostic manual) in
2013. This term was used to describe
Autistic people who were ‘higher functioning’.
Hans Asperger, for whom the syndrome was named, was connected with Nazi
race hygiene policies which were designed to kill disabled people. The idea that some autistic people who have
particular skillsets or attributes connected to their autism are superior to
other autistic people, or to other non-autistic people, is the basis for Aspie
Supremacy. Although Aspie Supremacy
isn’t well tolerated within the autistic community, there are nonetheless those
who prefer to dissociate from lesser socially acceptable forms of autism and
lean towards a form of supremacy which is firmly rooted in colonial
hierarchies. Heilker, (2012) says:
‘…I find this rhetoric disturbing, since any discourse that invokes a reference to genetic or evolutionary superiority will, of course, immediately raise the spectre of Nazi racism and Aryan eugenics. Here that association cements the idea of autistics as white.’
To further
explore the idea of white supremacy, I refer to Mills, (1997, p.1) who says
that ‘white supremacy is the unnamed political system that has made the world
what it is today.’ He goes on to say that
‘…the lack of appropriate concepts can hinder learning, interfere with memory,
block inferences, obstruct explanation, and perpetuate problems’ (Mills,
1997, p.7).
He refers also to feminist theory and the act of deconstructing some of the
dominant narratives. Within an
intersectional framework, the dominant narrative (which is predominantly white
and male), needs to be dismantled with an acute awareness of how the narrative
disables and unequalises different groupings to varying degrees, and sometimes
in a multiplicity of ways.
From an epistemic perspective autistic people experience injustice in many respects. Catala, Faucher and Poirier, (2021), speak of how ‘neuronormativity and neurotypical ignorance… give rise to a host of epistemic injustices for autistic people.’ They go on to define neuronormativity as
‘the prevalent, neurotypical set of assumptions, norms, and practices that construes neurotypicality as the sole acceptable or superior mode of cognition, and that stigmatizes attitudes, behaviors, or actions that reflect neuroatypical modes of cognition as deviant or inferior.’
The reference to
‘neurotypical ignorance' defers to Charles W. Mills and his concept of ‘white
ignorance’, the idea here being that neurotypical privilege leads to ignorance
about norms and assumptions which stigmatize autistic people.
Catala,
Faucher and Poirier, (2021) also speak of ‘testimonial quieting’ which occurs
when a hearer dismisses or invalidates a speaker’s testimony
pre-emptively. This occurs, as mentioned
above, in the areas of medical research and advocacy where organisations and
individuals position themselves as experts, yet exclude the voice of actually
autistic people.
Similarly Fricker,
(2007, p. 7), in speaking of hermeneutical injustice and its harm, speaks of a
‘…hermeneutical gap’ which ‘prevents them in particular from making sense of an experience which it is strongly in their interests to render intelligible.’
Hence autistic people are epistemically
disadvantaged in a number of ways; they are placed outside neuronormative
society which by its ignorance, is unaware of different ways of thinking, and
they are discredited from a testimonial perspective, in that their words and
accounts of their own experience is devalued.
4. Autism in
educational settings.
In educational settings, there are multiple facets to engaging with autistic individuals. Historically, girls and people of colour have not had access to the same level of diagnosis. This is due largely to the medical and societal bias which sees autists as white boys. Heilker (2012), speaks about this in terms of social perceptions and popular media:
‘our conceptions of autism and autistics are driven primarily by what we encounter in popular media. What we learn from the movies, for instance, is that all autistics are white.’
With regard to
the Traveller community in Ireland, at a Seanad Public Consultation Committee
debate on Tuesday, 9 Jul 2019, the topic of which was: Travellers Towards a More Equitable Ireland
Post-Recognition, Rose Marie Maughan spoke at length about autism in the
Traveller community and said that:
‘It is unclear from the statistics how many Traveller autistic children there are and the services they require. It is unclear whether the services are both clinically and culturally appropriate.’
Societal
perceptions, and the biases mentioned above, can lead to educators not
recognising the indicators of autism, and therefore not accommodating the
disability. Likewise, from an epistemic perspective, autistic individuals can
be diminished in the knowledge sphere, and their words (or non- speaking forms
of communication) can be dismissed. Outward
manifestations of autism or neurodivergence in general can be seen as aggression
or ‘trouble making’ tendencies, or sometimes, in the case of girls, as a lack
of focus or ‘daydreaming’. This lack of
recognition and awareness can create huge educational disadvantage for those
with autism. Similarly, in adult educational settings, norms around
communication styles are pervasive and likely to exclude autistic people.
Autistic individuals may have a more direct communication style, or perhaps may
be reluctant to participate in discussion because of previous experience of
invalidation, and being made to feel that their knowledge and testimony is not
valued at best, and at worst criticised or dismissed.
Miranda Fricker
(Fricker, 2007, p.159) describes this experience from an epistemic perspective:
‘That hermeneutical injustice most typically manifests itself in the speaker struggling to make herself intelligible in a testimonial exchange raises a grim possibility: that hermeneutical injustice might often be compounded by testimonial injustice …In such a case, the speaker is doubly wronged: once by the structural prejudice in the shared hermeneutical resource, and once by the hearer in making an identify-prejudiced credibility judgement.’
Claiming
autistic identity is enabled by access to information and understanding, as
well peer support. By the very nature of
how autism is diagnosed and the paradigm of management or treatment, many young
people may not be able to access pathways to fully embracing their autistic
identity. Even for those children who
are diagnosed, and who have affirming support, there is nonetheless a feeling
of being different, and an actual substantial difference in perceiving the
world, and in translating that world into manageable form for themselves. Within many social and educational settings,
autistic people are doubly disadvantaged: they are often perceived on an
individual level as being ‘weird’ and are also subject to the general societal
biases around autism. Furthermore, on a
physical level, sensory issues and other brain differences, such as, for example,
difficulties in transitioning from one task to another can create disadvantage
in learning environments. It is
important, also, to refer to autistic ‘masking’ in social and educational
settings. Masking is the term used
within the autistic community to describe the adoption of neurotypical
presentation, in order to conceal autistic identity. As described by Radulski, (2023), ‘this is where people conceal certain
traits and replace them with
neurotypical ones to avoid being recognised as neurominorities.’ This can take many forms such as increasing
eye contact, mirroring the body language or cadence of neurotypical people, and
restricting body movements. The
consequences of masking can be severe for autistic people but it’s a necessary
survival mechanism for many autists by virtue of the fact that, without it,
they may suffer social exclusion, lack of access to employment and in the case
of people of colour, there is a real risk of police violence (Radulski,
2023).
5. Pluralism and acceptance, the role of
recognising and accepting autistic identity.
The word ‘acceptance’ is an important one
for the autistic community and is part of a move towards changing from the
widely used ‘autism awareness’ to a more inclusive and less passive interaction. April, which is Autism Awareness Month, can
have an adverse impact on autistic advocates and autistic people in general, as
they listen to a plethora of voices speaking over them and see symbolism which
many autistic people find offensive, like the puzzle piece. (Autistic
Not Weird, 2020).
To once again use Tougaw’s (2020) words
‘cerebral pluralism’, it could be posited that this concept should be a
starting point on the road to societal acceptance of autistic and other
neurodivergent people. This kind of
pluralism should encompass a recognition of the autistic ontological state, and
would entail that neuronormative society comprehends the necessity of an essential
shift in thinking and an ability to self-reflect on potential bias. This is especially true when it comes to
communication in its different forms such as verbal, written, visual, via body
language, etc.
Milton, (2012) defined the ‘double empathy problem’ in reference to
communication, as follows:
‘The ‘double empathy problem’: a disjuncture in reciprocity between two differently disposed social actors which becomes more marked the wider the disjuncture in dispositional perceptions of the lifeworld – perceived as a breach in the ‘natural attitude’ of what constitutes ‘social reality’ for ‘non-autistic spectrum’ people and yet an everyday and often traumatic experience for ‘autistic people’.’
This communicative dissonance is a
substantial barrier for autistic people, with autistic directness oftentimes
being interpreted as rudeness, and likewise, some physical ways in which
autistic people communicate distress or attempt to self-regulate via stimming,
can be seen as disruptive or challenging.
Authentic inclusion would eliminate the need for autistic people to
‘perform’ as allistic people.
Another issue for autistic people is
the prevalence of ABA (Applied Behaviour Analysis). It is beyond the scope of this essay to
discuss in full, but this is a controversial area for the neurodivergent
community. Based on behaviour modification,
its aim is to make autistic children perform to neurotypical standards, by
using reward and punishment systems. ABA
was founded by Ivar Løvaas, who was a clinical psychologist and
also had strong links to gay conversion therapy. ABA has been strongly
criticised by a large number of autistic adults, who have recounted their own trauma
resulting from having experienced ABA, and it has been strongly critiqued for
having a very poor research basis.
(RodrÃguez Mega, 2023)
In conclusion,
the prevalence of non-autistic people occupying the autistic knowledge space
creates an ongoing epistemic injustice and inequality. Greater acceptance within a pluralistic
society requires that autistic voices are heard.
*Note: I have chosen to use neurodiversity affirming language, which is the preference of autistic people, throughout the text. For reference see Autistic Not Weird, (2022). This may not be reflected in some quotes and also is with the understanding that individual wishes with regard to terminology should be respected.
Bibliography
American Psychiatric Association (no date) What
Is Autism Spectrum Disorder? Available at: https://www.psychiatry.org/patients-families/autism/what-is-autism-spectrum-disorder (Accessed: 1 May 2023).
Autistic Not Weird (2020) Why Autism Awareness
Month Can Be Traumatising For Autistic People. Available at: https://autisticnotweird.com/awareness-month/ (Accessed: 11 May 2023).
Autistic Not Weird (2022) Results And Analysis
Of The Autistic Not Weird 2022 Autism Survey, Autistic Not
Weird . Available at: http://autisticnotweird.com/autismsurvey/?fbclid=IwAR3jiAHJUY1FJ8pjvWkWTSB (Accessed: 1 May 2023).
Autistic UK (2023) Neurodiversity | Autistic UK
CIC, Autistic UK CIC . Available at: https://www.autisticuk.org/neurodiversity (Accessed: 7 May 2023).
Catala, A., Faucher, L. and Poirier, P. (2021)
‘Autism, epistemic injustice, and epistemic disablement: a relational account
of epistemic agency’, Synthese, 199(3–4), pp. 9013–9039. doi: 10.1007/s11229-021-03192-7.
De Hooge, A. N. (2019) ‘Binary Boys: Autism, Aspie
Supremacy and Post/Humanist Normativity’, DSQ, 39(1). doi: 10.18061/dsq.v39i1.6461.
Dotson, K. (2011) ‘Tracking Epistemic Violence,
Tracking Practices of Silencing’, Hypatia, 26(2), pp. 236–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1527-2001.2011.01177.x.
Fricker, M. (2007) Epistemic Injustice.
Oxford University Press.
Heilker, P. (2012) ‘Autism, Rhetoric, and
Whiteness’, DSQ, 32(4). doi: 10.18061/dsq.v32i4.1756.
Hens, K., Robeyns, I. and Schaubroeck, K. (2021)
‘The Ethics of Autism’, Philosophy Compass . John Wiley & Sons Ltd.,
14(1). doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12559.
Irish Association of Speech and Language Therapists
and Psychological Society of Ireland (2023) Autism Innovation Strategy Joint
Submission. Psychological Society of Ireland. Available at: https://www.psychologicalsociety.ie/source/AutismInnovationStrategyfinal.pdf (Accessed: 1 May 2023).
Kemp, J. (2023) Navigating
Neurodiversity-Affirming Language, https://jenniferkemp.com.au/.
Available at: https://jenniferkemp.com.au/wp/wp-content/uploads/2023/02/resource-navigating-neurodiversity-affirming-language.pdf (Accessed: 1 May 2023).
Kourti, M. (2021) ‘A Critical Realist Approach on
Autism: Ontological and Epistemological Implications for Knowledge Production
in Autism Research’, Front. Psychol., 12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.713423.
Lindblom, A. (2017) Stepping out of the Shadows
of Colonialism to the Beat of the Drum: The meaning of music for five First
Nations children with autism in British Columbia, Canada. University of
Eastern Finland.
Maughan, R. M. (no date) The Rise Of The
Traveller Autistic Voice, Travellers Voice. Available at: https://www.travellersvoice.ie/2019/11/15/the-rise-of-the-traveller-autistic-voice/ (Accessed: 2 May 2023).
Mills, C. W. (1997) The racial contract.
Ithaca: Cornell University Press, p. 1, 7.
Milton, D. E. M. (2012) ‘On the ontological status
of autism: the “double empathy problem”’, Disability & Society,
27(6), pp. 883–887. doi: 10.1080/09687599.2012.710008.
Murray, S. (2008) Representing Autism.
Liverpool University Press.
Pellicano, L. et al. (2018) ‘A new era for
autism research, and for our journal’, Autism, 22(2), pp. 82–83. doi: 10.1177/1362361317748556.
Radulski, B. (2023) What Are ‘masking’ And
‘camouflaging’ In The Context Of Autism And ADHD?, The Conversation.
Available at: https://theconversation.com/what-are-masking-and-camouflaging-in-the-context-of-autism-and-adhd-193446 (Accessed: 11 May 2023).
RodrÃguez Mega, E. (2023) ‘“The best way to get it
right is to listen to us” — autistic people argue for a stronger voice in
research’, Nature, 617(7960), pp. 238–241. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-023-01549-1.
Shepard, N. (2010) Rewiring Difference and
Disability: Narratives of Asperger’s Syndrome in the Twenty-First Century, Bowling
Green State University . Available at: https://etd.ohiolink.edu/apexprod/rws_etd/send_file/send?accession=bgsu1276714818.
Tougaw, J. (2020) Neurodiversity: The Movement,
Psychology Today . Psychology Today. Available at: https://www.psychologytoday.com/ie/blog/the-elusive-brain/202004/neurodiversity-the-movement.